Dishion, T. J., McCord, J., & Poulin, F. (1999). When interventions harm: Peer groups and problem behavior. American psychologist, 54(9), 755.
Below is my summary of this paper
This article explored developmental and intervention evidence
relevant to iatrogenic effects (contagion effects) in peer-group interventions.
Contrary to historical assumptions about the beneficial effects of
friendships on children's social development, adolescence is also a time when
such relationships can undermine healthy development (see Hartup, 1996). We
have spent the past five years studying the subtle but powerful influence of
deviant friendships on escalations in problem behavior during adolescence. Much
of this research was conducted using the Oregon Youth Study (OYS) boys (Capaldi
& Patterson, 1987; Patterson, Reid, & Dishion, 1992).
1.
The factors relate to iatrogenic effects in
treatment
1) AGE
Additional analyses revealed that older (i.e., postpubertal)
youth, with the highest initial level of problem behavior, were most
susceptible to the iatrogenic effect (Poulin et al., in press).
Although the
intervention groups were closely supervised to prevent direct encouragement of
problem behavior, perusal of the videotapes suggests that the older children mobilized more
group attention than their younger, less deviant counterparts. Attention
in the group may have been elicited, not so much by the content of the
discussions, as by dress, behavior, and nonverbal expressions. These ideas will
be explored in future analyses
The data from
both intervention studies suggest that the older, more deviant children were the most vulnerable
to iatrogenic effects from peer aggregation. This fact is consistent
with recent developmental research on the influence of friends.
2)
The level of
delinquency
In early adolescence, youth with moderate levels
of delinquency, and who had deviant friends, were those who escalated to more
serious forms of antisocial behavior (Coie, Miller-Johnson, Terry, Maumary-Gremaud,
& Lochman, 1996; Vitaro, Tremblay, Kerr, Pagani, & Bukowski, 1997).
Poulin, Dishion, and Haas (1999) found that boys with the poorest relationships
and highest delinquency were most vulnerable to deviancy training, with
respect to increasing
delinquent behavior
2.
The mechanism of iatrogenic effects
We offer two
possible processes that might explain the converging evidence from intervention
and developmental research on the influence of peers on social development: (a)
youth being actively reinforced through laughter, social attention, and
interest for deviant behavior are likely to increase such behavior (Dishion et
al., 1995; Dishion et al., 1997; Dishion, Spracklen, et al., 1996); and (b)
high-risk adolescents derive meaning and values from the deviancy training
process that provides the cognitive basis for motivation to commit delinquent
acts in the future (i.e., construct theory; McCord, 1997, 1999) .
Both processes
suggest that repetition of contact within the peer-group intervention would
create the iatrogenic effect observed in these two intervention studies,
especially among those youth likely to engage in deviant talk and behavior
primarily in the company of peers. We hypothesize that the reinforcement
processes within the peer groups are quite subtle and potentially powerful. For
example, Buehler, Patterson, and Furniss (1966) found that within institutional
settings, peers provided a rate of reinforcement of 9-to-l, compared with adult
staff, suggesting that the density of reinforcement from peers can be so high
it seriously undermines adult guidance.
In our
analysis of therapist and client behavior predicting the magnitude of the
iatrogenic effect, we found that observer impressions of therapist effectiveness were positively associated
with growth in subsequent problem behavior (Dishion, Poulin, Hunt, and
Van Male, 1998).
3.
What can we do
Based on the
studies reviewed, there is reason to be cautious and to avoid aggregating young
high-risk adolescents into intervention groups: Some conditions might further
exacerbate the iatrogenic effect. The age of the child is certainly relevant, as younger and older
children may be less affected by the processes described above. For
example, a peer-training program, in which boys in third through fifth grades
were trained to attend to behavioral cues of intentions, reduced the amount of
aggression displayed by aggressive, unpopular boys (Hudley & Graham, 1993).
A two-year
program, combining family interventions with peer training of boys identified
as aggressive by their kindergarten teachers also resulted in reduced
antisocial behavior and increased school success in subsequent years
(McCord, Tremblay,
Vitaro, & Desmarais-Gervais, 1994; Tremblay, Pagani-Kurtz, Masse, Vitaro,
& Pihl, 1995).
However, these
peer-training groups were designed to include a mix of prosocial and aggressive youth, which may
be the desired strategy. Feldman (1992) also found that mixing antisocial youth with prosocial youth in
interventions was an effective strategy in reducing their problem
behavior. Aggregation of high-risk youth, then, may be helpful in middle
childhood or when groups also enforce interactions with prosocial children who do not respond with
interest to talk of deviance.
Age of the child and format of the peer
aggregation may impact the risk of producing negative effects on problem behavior.
Another factor
to influence the risk of an iatrogenic effect is the kinds of youth included in
the groups. Peer aggregation of depressed adolescents into cognitive behavioral
interventions, for example, produces positive effects and
statistically
reliable
reductions in adolescent depression (Lewinsohn & Clark, 1990). Of note,
however, is that in this research, youth with comorbid disruptive behavior
disorders were not included in the study. Therefore, interventions aggregating
youth in the treatment of depression, including those with antisocial behavior,
may unwittingly produce increases in problem behavior. A broad view of the
developmental and intervention literature would suggest that early adolescence
is an especially vulnerable time for peer effects on social development, at
least for children at high risk for delinquency.
Research by
Chamberlain and colleagues revealed that mobilizing adult caregiving is a
critical and viable intervention target for even the most severe adolescent
delinquent (Chamberlain & Moore, 1998; Chamberlain & Reid, 1998). Her
research compared a treatment foster care model with group home treatment,
finding that the former resulted in reductions in deviant peer contact and
subsequent self-reported and court-documented delinquency, compared with group
home placement.
This is a really interesting article. Especially when you consider how we typically respond to children displaying antisocial behavior. Which is generally to separate them from their prosocial peers and in the delinquent cases and foster case from their parents or caregivers. When the research suggests that we should be finding interventions that integrate them with prosocial peers and that bolster parent and caregiver engagement.
ReplyDelete